首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   120篇
  免费   4篇
教育   100篇
科学研究   1篇
各国文化   3篇
体育   2篇
文化理论   1篇
信息传播   17篇
  2023年   2篇
  2021年   1篇
  2020年   4篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   2篇
  2017年   9篇
  2016年   7篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   6篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   1篇
  2011年   2篇
  2010年   1篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   4篇
  2007年   2篇
  2006年   2篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   1篇
  2001年   1篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   1篇
  1997年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1991年   2篇
  1989年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   2篇
  1971年   2篇
  1969年   1篇
  1955年   1篇
排序方式: 共有124条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
The Council for the Study of Community Colleges (CSCC) celebrated its 50th anniversary in 2008. This article summarizes the establishment of the Council and sets it in the context of the development of the community college movement. Highlights of research conducted by CSCC members to address contemporary issues facing community colleges are described. A summary of insights offered by members during a 2008 plenary session, research presented at the 50th annual conference, and key community college leaders are described in the context of the historical evolution of community college research and future directions.  相似文献   
52.
53.
The purpose of this research was to investigate the cognitive abilities that explain reading comprehension across childhood and early adulthood. Drawing from the standardization sample of the Woodcock–Johnson III, analyses were conducted with large samples at age levels spanning early childhood to early adulthood: 5 to 6 (n = 639), 7 to 8 (n = 720), 9 to 13 (n = 1,995), 14 to 19 (n = 1,615), and 20 to 39 (n = 1,409). Using a model including factors representing general intelligence, Cattell–Horn–Carroll broad abilities, and reading decoding skills, results revealed significant direct effects for reading decoding skills and Crystallized Intelligence on reading comprehension across all age levels. Memory‐related abilities, processing speed, and auditory processing demonstrated indirect effects on reading comprehension through reading decoding skills. The magnitude of direct and indirect effects varied as a function of age. The results provide support for integrative models of reading that include both direct and indirect effects of cognitive abilities on reading comprehension and for consideration of developmental differences in the cognitive aptitudes predicting reading comprehension. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.  相似文献   
54.
DRAMATIC SOUNDINGS: EVALUATIONS AND RETRACTIONS CULLED FROM 30 YEARS OF DRAMATIC CRITICISM. By John Gassner. Introduction and editing by Glenn Loney. New York: Crown Publishers, 1968; pp. xx+716. $7.50.

BEN JONSON'S ‘DOTAGES’: A RECONSIDERATION OF THE LATE PLAYS. By Larry S. Champion. Lexington: University of Kentucky Press, 1967; pp. viii+156. $6.50.

LOPE DE VEGA. By Francis C. Hayes. (Twayne's World Authors Series, no. 28.) New York: Twayne Publishers, 1967; pp. 160. $4.50.

THE FORMAL FRENCH. By W. L. Wiley. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1967; pp. xii+317. $6.75.

THE BAROQUE THEATRE: A CULTURAL HISTORY OF THE 17th AND 18th CENTURIES. By Margarete Baur‐Heinhold. Photographs by Helga Schmidt‐Glassner. Translated by Mary Whittall. New York: McGraw‐Hill, 1967; pp. 292. $32.00.

THEATER DES BAROCK: KULTURGESCHICHTE IN EINZELDARSTELLUNGEN. By Margarete Baur‐Heinhold. München: Verlag Georg D. W. Callwey, 1966; pp. 296. DM 88,00.

RUSSIAN COMEDY, 1765–1823. By David J. Welsh. (Slavistic Printings and Reprintings, LXV.) New York: Humanities Press, 1966; pp. 133. $6.50.

THE RISE AND FALL OF THE WELL‐MADE PLAY. By John Russell Taylor. New York: Hill and Wang, 1967; pp. 175. $5.75.

IONESCO AND GENET: PLAYWRIGHTS OF SILENCE. By Josephine Jacobsen and William R. Mueller. New York: Hill and Wang, 1968; pp. xii+242. $5.95; paper $1.95.

THE PLAYWRIGHTS SPEAK. Edited by Walter Wager. Introduction by Harold Clurman. New York: Delacorte Press, 1967; pp. xxx+290. $6.00.

OCCUPATIONAL COSTUME IN ENGLAND FROM THE 11th CENTURY TO 1914. By Phillis Cunnington and Catherine Lucas, with chapters by Alan Mansfield. New York: Barnes and Noble, 1967; pp. 427. $10.00.

THE PRIMAL CURSE: THE MYTH OF CAIN AND ABEL IN THE THEATRE. By Honor Matthews. New York: Schocken Books, 1967; pp. 221. $5.95.

THE POETIC TRADITION: ESSAYS ON GREEK, LATIN, AND ENGLISH POETRY. Edited by Don Cameron Allen and Henry T. Rowell. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1968; pp. ix+142. $5.95.

LATIN POETRY: THE AGE OF RHETORIC AND SATIRE. By Clarence W. Mendell. Hamden, Conn.: Archon Books, 1967; pp. viii+223. $6.000.

THE FICTIONS OF SATIRE. By Ronald Paulson. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1967; pp. viii+228. $7.00.

CONCEPTIONS OF REALITY IN MODERN AMERICAN POETRY. By L. S. Dembo. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1966; pp. ix+248. $6.00.

WHY LITERARY CRITICISM IS NOT AN EXACT SCIENCE. By Harry Levin. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1967; pp. 27. Paper $1.00.

DEMOSTHENES’ ON THE CROWN: A CRITICAL CASE STUDY OF A MASTERPIECE OF ANCIENT ORATORY. Edited by James J. Murphy with a new translation by John J. Keaney. (Studies in Speech, No. 7.) New York: Random House, 1967; pp. ix+208. Paper $2.25.

THE RHETORIC OF PASCAL: A STUDY OF HIS ART OF PERSUASION IN THE ‘PROVINCIALES’ AND THE ‘PENSÉES.’ By Patricia Topliss. New York: Humanities Press, 1966; pp. 342. $8.50.

A CRITIQUE OF THE NEW COMMONPLACES. By Jacques Ellul. Translated by Helen Weaver. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1968; pp. vii+306. $6.95.

TRADITION AND DISSENT: A RHETORIC‐READER. By Florence Bonzer Greenberg and Anne P. Heffley. Indianapolis: Bobbs‐Merrill, 1967; pp. xviii+426. Paper $3.95.

THE CORRESPONDENCE OF EDMUND BURKE: VOLUME VI, JULY 1789‐DECEMBER 1791. Edited by Alfred Cobban and Robert A. Smith. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1967; pp. xxvi+495. $13.50.

GEORGE WASHINGTON IN THE AMERICAN REVOLUTION, 1775–1783. By James Thomas Flexner. Boston: Little, Brown, 1968; pp. xvii+599. $10.00

THE DIARY OF JAMES A. GARFIELD. Vols. I and II, 1848–1874. Edited by Harry James Brown and Frederick D. Williams. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press, 1967; pp. lxx+496 and 450. $30.00.

WASHINGTON GLADDEN: PROPHET OF THE SOCIAL GOSPEL. By Jacob Henry Dorn. Columbus: Ohio State University Press, 1968; pp. x+489. $8.00.

FDR: ARCHITECT OF AN ERA. By Rexford G. Tugwell. New York: Macmillan, 1967; pp. xviii+270. $4.95.

CRISIS IN CREDIBILITY. By Bruce Ladd. New York: New American Library, 1968; pp. vi+247. $5.50.

ANYTHING BUT THE TRUTH: THE CREDIBILITY GAP—HOW THE NEWS IS MANAGED IN WASHINGTON. By William McGaffin and Erwin Knoll. New York: G. P. Putnam's Sons, 1968; pp. 250. $7.95.

THE GREAT SOCIETY READER: THE FAILURE OF AMERICAN LIBERALISM. Edited by Marvin Gettleman and David Mermelstein. New York: Random House, 1967; pp. xiv+551. $8.95; paper $2.45.

THE SPEECHES OF MALCOLM X AT HARVARD. Edited with an introduction by Archie Epps. New York: William Morrow, 1968; pp. 191. $4.95.

SOUNDS OF THE STRUGGLE: PERSONS AND PERSPECTIVES IN CIVIL RIGHTS. By C. Eric Lincoln. New York: William Morrow, 1967; pp. 252. $5.00.

THE COMPUTER AND INVASION OF PRIVACY. U. S. House of Representatives, Subcommittee of the Committee on Government Operations, 89th Congress, Second Session, 1966. New York: Arno Press, 1967; pp. iv+311. $4.95.

THE PRESENCE OF THE WORD: SOME PROLEGOMENA FOR CULTURAL AND RELIGIOUS HISTORY. By Walter J. Ong, S.J. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1967; pp. xiv+360. $6.95.

RADIO PROGRAMMING IN ACTION: REALITIES AND OPPORTUNITIES. Edited by Sherril W. Taylor. New York: Hastings House, 1967; pp. 183. $6.50.

RADIO BROADCASTING: AN INTRODUCTION TO THE SOUND MEDIUM. Edited by Robert L. Hilliard. New York: Hastings House, 1967; pp. 190. $6.95; paper $4.40.

VERBAL BEHAVIOR AND GENERAL BEHAVIOR THEORY. Edited by David L. Horton and Theodore R. Dixon. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice‐Hall, 1968; pp. ix+396. $9.95.

EXPERIMENTS IN PERSUASION. Edited by Ralph L. Rosnow and Edward J. Robinson. New York: Academic Press, 1967; pp. xix+519. $8.95.

THE LONDON SCHOOL OF LINGUISTICS: A STUDY OF THE LINGUISTIC THEORIES OF B. MALINOWSKI AND J. R. FIRTH. By D. Terence Langendoen. (Research Monograph, No. 46.) Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, 1968; pp. xii+123. $5.95.

THE MODIFICATION OF STUTTERING. By Eugene J. Brutten and Donald J. Shoemaker. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice‐Hall, 1967; pp. xii+148. $5.50.

PRE‐GLOTTALIZATION IN ENGLISH STANDARD PRONUNCIATION. By Bjørn Stâlhane Andrésen. (Norwegian Studies in English, No. 13.) New York: Humanities Press, 1968; pp. 187. $6.75.

FORMS OF ENGLISH: ACCENT, MORPHEME, ORDER. By Dwight L. Bolinger. Edited by Isamu Abe and Tetsuya Kanekiyo. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1965: pp. ix+334. $7.95.  相似文献   
55.
Although several investigations have addressed the nature of communication in men's relationships with their sons, relatively few have focused on positive interaction patterns, such as the exchange of affection, even though affection is of considerable importance to relational maintenance and satisfaction. According to affection exchange theory, affection is such a valuable relational resource because of the contributions it makes to humans’ long‐term viability and reproductive success. As such a resource, it should, thus, be governed by adaptive motivations, among which is the motivation for parents to invest in their children in ways that maximize their long‐term evolutionary success. Using the theory of discriminative parental solicitude, we predicted differences in the amount of affection men communicate to their biological sons, adopted sons, and step‐sons. We tested our predictions in two studies involving a total of 384 males. We discuss implications of the results for explaining the superordinate evolutionary motivations governing affectionate communication.  相似文献   
56.
The study evaluates how marriage and the parenting alliance affect parenting experiences over time. Couples ( N = 79) with school-age children who have mental retardation completed self-report and observational measures of marriage, the parenting alliance, and parenting attitudes and behaviors at 2 periods, 18–24 months apart. Longitudinal structural equation modeling demonstrated significant effects of marital quality on changes over time in self-reports of perceived parenting competence for both the mothers and the fathers, and in observed negative mother-child interactions. Also, in all cases, the parenting alliance mediated the effects of marriage on parenting experiences. There was little evidence of reciprocal causation in which parenting variables predicted change in the quality of marriage and the parenting alliance. Interactions involving child age suggested that teenagers as opposed to younger children were more reactive to negative features of their parents' marital functioning and parenting alliance. Implications are discussed regarding stable but negative marital functioning and regarding possible differences in mothers' and fathers' parenting in the context of marital distress.  相似文献   
57.
It is often assumed that parents completing behavior rating scales during the assessment of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) can deliberately manipulate the outcomes of the assessment. To detect these actions, items designed to detect over‐reporting or under‐reporting of results are sometimes embedded in such rating scales. This study presents the results of an experimental study in which parents (a) read a scenario telling them that their hypothetical son's teacher has suggested their son may have ADHD and (b) considered assigned goals for the assessment. Parents then completed the accompanying Conners 3Parent Short form (Conners 3‐P[S]) in a manner that they believed would achieve their assigned goals. Findings showed that parents are able to engage in deception when completing behavior rating scales. The validity scales embedded in the Conners 3‐P(S), however, demonstrated mixed results for detecting parental deception with the Negative Impression validity scale, accurately detecting attempts to malinger in the majority of cases, whereas the Positive Impression validity scale appears to have little to no diagnostic utility for the detection of defensive responding. Clinicians utilizing behavior rating scales should carefully consider results, and nonresults, obtained from embedded validity scales when interpreting parent responses to behavior rating scales as part of an ADHD assessment.  相似文献   
58.
59.
Summary The above catalog contains fifteen headings, each of which indicates a collection of families of models for multiplication and division of whole numbers. The catalog refers to somewhat more than sixteen families of models which are easily distinguished one from the other.Not included in the catalog thus far developed are several interpretations of multiplication and division that are also of interest. Among these are models based on the equivalency of denominations of money and various units of measurement. Other interpretations which are of historical interest are those of McLellan and Dewey [15] and Thorndike [24]. The relation between models of operations on whole numbers and models of operations defined on larger universal sets is also of interest. One aspect of this area of interest is the process of constructing models of multiplication and division of whole numbers from such models by altering the rules of the model or delimiting its universal set. For example, one can begin with one of Diénès' models of multiplication of integers [8, pp. 57–58] and make approapriate adjustments and result in a model of multiplication of whole numbers. Other interpretations developed by Diénès are of interest because they involve concretizations of whole numbers which are operators as opposed to states [8, pp. 12, 30; 9, p, 36].These are a great many strategies available for the use of models in teaching the operations on whole numbers. In one such strategy, an educator can define either multiplication or division on some basis (most likely in terms of a model) and then the other can be defined as its inverse.Another strategy is to define each operation in terms of a different model. For example, one might define multiplication in terms of the repeated addition model and division in terms of the repeated subtraction model.Still another type of procedure involves a multiple embodiment strategy in which several interpretations are taught as representing each operation.The choice of a particular strategy would depend upon a great many factors. Some of the factors would be the type of culture and students for which the program is written, the psychological assumptions adopted by the writer, and the writer's knowledge of the domain of models for the operations as well as their relation to the abstract mathematical domain which they represent. This article has contributed to a basis for intelligent decisions in this area by presenting a characterization of the domain of models for multiplication and division of whole numbers and their relation to the abstract operations.  相似文献   
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号