首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   24225篇
  免费   201篇
  国内免费   21篇
教育   17721篇
科学研究   2662篇
各国文化   125篇
体育   1466篇
综合类   6篇
文化理论   470篇
信息传播   1997篇
  2021年   138篇
  2020年   195篇
  2019年   269篇
  2018年   2453篇
  2017年   2359篇
  2016年   1869篇
  2015年   341篇
  2014年   425篇
  2013年   2371篇
  2012年   500篇
  2011年   980篇
  2010年   1089篇
  2009年   631篇
  2008年   883篇
  2007年   1351篇
  2006年   297篇
  2005年   588篇
  2004年   657篇
  2003年   553篇
  2002年   300篇
  2001年   448篇
  2000年   309篇
  1999年   253篇
  1998年   117篇
  1997年   166篇
  1996年   132篇
  1995年   123篇
  1994年   144篇
  1993年   110篇
  1992年   181篇
  1991年   197篇
  1990年   192篇
  1989年   186篇
  1988年   153篇
  1987年   153篇
  1986年   153篇
  1985年   193篇
  1984年   150篇
  1983年   126篇
  1982年   120篇
  1981年   113篇
  1980年   107篇
  1979年   154篇
  1978年   119篇
  1977年   105篇
  1976年   78篇
  1975年   104篇
  1974年   95篇
  1973年   101篇
  1971年   85篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
62.
Moral minds     
Steve Clarke 《Minerva》2008,46(1):147-150
  相似文献   
63.
The purpose of this study was to examine if steps/day taken by middle school students varied based on aerobic fitness classification. Middle school students (N = 223; 112 girls, 111 boys) were assigned to three aerobic fitness categories (HIGH, MOD, LOW) based on results of the FlTNESSGRAM PACER test. Four weekdays of pedometer monitoring determined activity levels (steps/day). Boys accumulated significantly more steps/day than girls, 11,589 +/- 3,270 and 10,232 +/- 2,517 steps/day, respectively; F(1,219) = 16.0, p < .001, eta2 = .055. There were no differences in steps/day between grades. HIGH fit participants accumulated significantly more steps/day, F(2, 217) = 12.2, p < .101, eta2 = .101, than moderately fit and low fit participants (approximately 1,491 and ; 2,867 steps/day, respectively). Middle school students who participated in sports in addition to physical education (PE) accumulated significantly more steps/day (approximately 980 steps/day) than those participating in PE only, F(1, 219) = 10.0, p < .01, eta2 = .044. Although the relationship between physical activity and aerobic fitness was moderate (0.35; p < .01), these data demonstrated significant differences in accumulated steps/day among youth of varying aerobic fitness levels. Whether improved fitness levels were the result of additional activity or the cause of it remains to be determined. Regardless, the fittest middle school students were also the most active and accumulated a significant amount of steps/day through organized extracurricular physical activities.  相似文献   
64.
65.
66.
67.
68.
The aim was to examine cross-cultural variation in linguistic responsiveness to young children in 10 English-speaking mother–child dyads and 10 Italian-speaking mother– child dyads. All 20 children were late talkers who possessed delays in expressive vocabulary development but age-appropriate cognitive and receptive language skills. Dyads were filmed in 15 minute free play contexts, which were transcribed and coded for measures of maternal linguistic input (e.g. rate, MLU, labels, expansions) and child language productivity (e.g. utterances, different words used). The results revealed that the Italian mothers used more utterances, spoke more quickly and used a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian mothers. The Italian children mirrored their mothers and also used more utterances and a more diverse vocabulary than the Canadian children. Mothers in both groups used similar percentages of responsive labels and expansions. However, Italian mothers responded to fewer of their children's vocalisations, using a smaller percentage of imitations and interpretations than the Canadian mothers. Correlations between maternal input and children's language productivity revealed that contingent language measures (e.g. imitations, interpretations, expansions) were related to high levels of productivity in children in both cultural groups. The results support the use of language interventions based on increasing maternal responsiveness for these children at the one-word stage of language development. They also point to differences that may be culturally based. For example, Italian mothers use faster rates of interaction and appear to have higher expectations for their children's verbal participation in interaction. This is reflected in higher rates of language production from their children, even though children in both cultural groups have similar vocabulary sizes.  相似文献   
69.
70.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号