首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1073篇
  免费   4篇
  国内免费   9篇
教育   849篇
科学研究   78篇
各国文化   5篇
体育   52篇
综合类   22篇
文化理论   2篇
信息传播   78篇
  2023年   11篇
  2022年   20篇
  2021年   39篇
  2020年   42篇
  2019年   29篇
  2018年   34篇
  2017年   42篇
  2016年   38篇
  2015年   25篇
  2014年   79篇
  2013年   127篇
  2012年   81篇
  2011年   74篇
  2010年   55篇
  2009年   96篇
  2008年   78篇
  2007年   68篇
  2006年   42篇
  2005年   25篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   20篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   7篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   2篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1086条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
51.
Learning from direct instruction can be enhanced by preparatory invention tasks: students invent an index that allows to differentiate a set of cases regarding important aspects (self-regulated). However, contradictory results have been found. As self-regulated activities often need practice, we tested whether the contradictory findings persist when students can practice inventing. We randomly assigned 99 eighth-grade students to two conditions (independent variable): they either invented twice (self-regulated; n = 49) or worked through worked solutions of the two tasks (guided; n = 50) before learning about ratios in physics from a lecture. Extraneous load, deep-structure recall, knowledge-gap perception, and self-efficacy were potential mediators. Transfer was the dependent measure. Guidance led to less extraneous load. However, self-regulation led to higher transfer because the students devoted more attention to the deep structure of the preparation tasks. Our findings suggest that—given some practice—self-regulated outperforms guided preparation for learning from direct instruction.  相似文献   
52.
We investigated whether the valence of performance feedback provided after a task, would affect participants’ perceptions of how much mental effort they invested in that same task. In three experiments, we presented participants with problem-solving tasks and manipulated the presence and valence of feedback between conditions (no, positive, or negative feedback valence), prior to asking them to rate how much mental effort they invested in solving that problem. Across the three experiments–with different problem-solving tasks and participant populations–we found that subjective ratings of effort investment were significantly higher after negative than after positive feedback; ratings given without feedback fell in between. These findings show that feedback valence alters perceived effort investment (possibly via task perceptions or affect), which can be problematic when effort is measured as an indicator of cognitive load. Therefore, it seems advisable to measure mental effort directly after each task, before giving feedback on performance.  相似文献   
53.
This research aimed at identifying unique effects of reading motivation on reading performance when controlling for cognitive skills, familial, and demographic background. We drew upon a longitudinal sample of N = 1508 secondary school students from 5th to 8th grade. Two types of intrinsic reading motivation (reading enjoyment, reading for interest), one type of extrinsic reading motivation (competition), and reading self-concept were measured by self-report questionnaires. Cognitive skills (reasoning, decoding speed) and reading performance were assessed using standardized tests and background variables were collected using student and parent questionnaires. Applying latent growth curve modeling, positive unique effects of reading enjoyment and reading self-concept and a negative unique effect of competition on the initial level of reading performance were recorded. Moreover, a positive unique effect of reading for interest on reading performance growth was recorded. One may conclude that enhancing students’ interest might be fruitful in terms of nurturing reading performance.  相似文献   
54.

Objective

To develop further the understanding of emotional abuse and neglect.

Methods

Building on previous work, this paper describes the further development of a conceptual framework for the recognition and management of emotional abuse and neglect. Training in this framework is currently being evaluated. The paper also briefly reviews more recent work on aspects of the definition, harm caused by emotional abuse and neglect and threshold.

Results

The paper arrives at a working definition as ‘persistent, non-physical, harmful interactions with the child by the caregiver, which include both commission and omission.’ There are many forms of harmful caregiver-child interactions, which can be placed in five categories, each category reflecting the fulfillment of one of the child's basic psycho-social needs and requiring a different therapeutic approach for its alleviation. The caregiver-child relationship is embedded within a psycho-social context. It is suggested that greater clarity can be gained about the child and family when information is sorted into the appropriate tiers of concerns: Tier 0 – Social & environmental factors, Tier I – Caregiver risk factors, Tier II – Caregiver-child interactions and Tier III – Child's functioning. It is further suggested that while intervention is required, this is directed towards protection, rather than providing immediate protection of the child. The work takes the form of a time-limited trial of therapeutic work to gauge the capacity of the caregivers to change. This initial work focuses Tiers 0–II. Statutory steps might be required in order to encourage the caregivers to engage. If insufficient progress is achieved, active child protection may be required which might include placing the child in an alternative family. However, some, usually older, children will remain in the emotionally abusive environment and they will require ongoing help and support.

Conclusion

While a greater understanding of emotional abuse and neglect is now possible, further evaluation of the utility of this framework is suggested.  相似文献   
55.
56.
以多民族地区的贵州省青少年体质健康行为模式为研究对象,通过问卷调查、访谈、实地考察等研究方法深入调查分析民族地区青少年体质健康特征、相关行为情况,得出导致青少年体质健康问题不仅有学校教育因素,同时也有家庭、社会等因素的结论。[1]根据这些因素探索整合学校、家庭、社会等相关资源的综合性干预措施。  相似文献   
57.
隐喻是一种重要的认知模式,是从一个范畴域向另一个范畴域的结构映射。相似性是隐喻的基础,也是隐喻区别于其他相关语言用法的重要条件。隐喻相似性依赖于客观世界和主观认知之间的互相作用,是可以创造的。相似性实际上仅表现于两个事物之间某一个方面或某一个点,是潜在的,是人类在这两个具有差异性的事物中突显其相似面或相似点的结果。  相似文献   
58.
规则空间是一种用来识别被试认知错误类型的数学模型,但其分类标准(参照点)一直依赖专家根据经验在测试前手工确定,使用难度太高,无法大规模普及使用。本文使用模糊聚类方法在测试后辅助确定规则空间中的参考点,使得普通教育工作者也能借助计算机使用规则空间模型。  相似文献   
59.
从模因-顺应视域来探究大学生网络暴力语言,并对这一语言现象背后产生的内在认知心理工作机制做出合理的认知语用解释,这是一种新的尝试。本研究发现大学生网络暴力语言的内在心理工作机制是被选择的强势模因对心理世界、物理世界、社交世界以及语言结构和交际目的顺应的结果。  相似文献   
60.
隐喻在语言的发展和使用中起着重要的作用。它不仅是一种修辞手法,而且是一种认知方式。文章从认知的视角探讨隐喻的构词扩义功能和语篇衔接功能,并提出把隐喻功能应用到英语专业语言课上,让学生依靠丰富的联想和想象去记忆使用词汇,提高创造性思维能力,从而更加透彻地理解语言并创造性地使用语言,提高语言语用能力。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号