首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   22826篇
  免费   108篇
  国内免费   9篇
教育   16325篇
科学研究   2678篇
各国文化   139篇
体育   1160篇
综合类   6篇
文化理论   503篇
信息传播   2132篇
  2020年   117篇
  2019年   179篇
  2018年   2388篇
  2017年   2354篇
  2016年   1827篇
  2015年   282篇
  2014年   333篇
  2013年   2356篇
  2012年   434篇
  2011年   966篇
  2010年   1036篇
  2009年   609篇
  2008年   873篇
  2007年   1395篇
  2006年   255篇
  2005年   557篇
  2004年   615篇
  2003年   521篇
  2002年   271篇
  2001年   141篇
  2000年   193篇
  1999年   133篇
  1998年   95篇
  1997年   107篇
  1996年   92篇
  1995年   83篇
  1994年   104篇
  1993年   122篇
  1992年   146篇
  1991年   170篇
  1990年   156篇
  1989年   155篇
  1988年   132篇
  1987年   140篇
  1986年   151篇
  1985年   158篇
  1984年   144篇
  1983年   147篇
  1982年   117篇
  1981年   126篇
  1980年   100篇
  1979年   146篇
  1978年   147篇
  1977年   102篇
  1976年   104篇
  1975年   89篇
  1974年   79篇
  1973年   95篇
  1972年   82篇
  1970年   80篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
David Kaldewey 《Minerva》2018,56(2):161-182
This article analyzes the concept of “grand challenges” as part of a shift in how scientists and policymakers frame and communicate their respective agendas. The history of the grand challenges discourse helps to understand how identity work in science and science policy has been transformed in recent decades. Furthermore, the question is raised whether this discourse is only an indicator, or also a factor in this transformation. Building on conceptual history and historical semantics, the two parts of the article reconstruct two discursive shifts. First, the observation that in scientific communication references to “problems” are increasingly substituted by references to “challenges” indicates a broader cultural trend of how attitudes towards what is problematic have shifted in the last decades. Second, as the grand challenges discourse is rooted in the sphere of sports and competition, it introduces a specific new set of societal values and practices into the spheres of science and technology. The article concludes that this process can be characterized as the sportification of science, which contributes to self-mobilization and, ultimately, to self-optimization of the participating scientists, engineers, and policymakers.  相似文献   
2.
3.
Letters     
  相似文献   
4.
Editorial     
B. Sury 《Resonance》2018,23(7):723-725
  相似文献   
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
Children are sensitive to a number of considerations influencing distributions of resources, including equality, equity, and reciprocity. We tested whether children use a specific type of reciprocity norm—market norms—in which resources are distributed differentially based strictly on amount offered in return. In two studies, 195 children 5–10 years and 60 adults distributed stickers to friends offering same or different amounts of money. Overall, participants distributed more equally when offers were the same and more unequally when offers were different. Although sensitive to why friends offered different amounts of money, children increasingly incorporated market norms into their distributions with age, as the oldest children and adults distributed more to those offering more, irrespective of the reasons provided.  相似文献   
10.
As a group, students with learning disabilities (LD) have social difficulties. One possible explanation for these difficulties is the unique way they process social information. Although students with LD may differ from their nondisabled peers in their social cognition, investigators have suggested the presence of subgroups within the population of students with LD who may differ in their social competence and, thereby, shed light on the source of the difficulties. The present exploratory study examined how two subgroups of students with LD in inclusive settings, students with high and low social status, perceive social situations. Using a sociometric technique, three students with LD receiving high social‐status ratings and three students with LD receiving low social‐status ratings were identified. A qualitative approach was used to gather and evaluate data from the participants and their teachers. Results suggested differences between the two subgroups in their (1) sensitivity to cues in the environment, (2) interpretation of social situations in relation to their own experiences, and (3) levels of self‐control. Implications of these findings for practice and future research are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号