首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到19条相似文献,搜索用时 343 毫秒
1.
拟采用迁移范式,以事件相关电位(ERP)成分N2、P3作为主要指标,结合不同迁移方式和不同学习"方式"探讨学习者的知识表征方式的差异。结果发现:内隐组和外显组被试在习得了一定的序列知识后,相对于标准序列的反应时,效应器迁移序列的反应时明显变慢;在脑电数据中,以N2的平均波幅作为指标,不管是内隐组还是外显组,标准序列与刺激迁移序列不存在区别,而与效应器迁移序列却差异显著。对于P3的平均波幅的分析得出:在内隐组,标准序列与效应器迁移序列不存在区别;而在外显组,两者却差异显著。该结果说明:效应器水平的编码机制是参与序列知识的学习的;且相对于知觉编码,不管以何种"方式"学习,效应器编码起的作用都要更大。  相似文献   

2.
针对效应器在动作序列学习中的作用机制主要存在2 种观点:动作序列学习依存于特定的效应器和动作序列学习独立于特定的效应器。研究 选取大学生为被试,在考虑性别均衡的前提下随机分为3 个试验组;采用序列反应时任务范式,设计反应迁移测验、刺激迁移测验和反应-刺激迁移 测验3 种测验任务,以学习迁移率为指标,分析动作序列学习的心理表征机制。结果发现:(1)在序列反应时任务中所发生的内隐学习,在反应迁移测 验、刺激迁移测验和反应-刺激迁移测验中均发生了学习迁移现象,平均迁移率分别为70.00%、50.79%和47.96%;(2)通过反应迁移测验、刺激迁移 测验和反应-刺激迁移测验中的迁移率比较表明,动作序列学习中独立于特定效应器的成分更占主导地位;(3)动作序列学习中知觉学习和动觉学 习同时存在,但这2 种学习又不是彼此独立的,不能将2 种成分进行简单的叠加。表明:动作序列学习中应重视和充分发挥技能学习的迁移规律,充 分发挥知觉学习、动觉学习的协同作用;动作序列技能的训练不必拘泥于单一动作效应器的训练,可以通过动作示范、变换不同效应器等策略,降低 因重复训练带来的情绪困扰、心理或生理疲劳。  相似文献   

3.
运动技能学习是内隐与外显协同学习在真实情境下表现较多的领域之一。作为一种行之有效的内隐学习方法,类比学习的研究局限于理论探讨和分离性研究,将类比学习和外显学习结合起来的研究较为鲜见。以初学者为受试,采用双因素实验设计,探讨不同年龄青少年网球初学者学习网球双手反手击球技能时类比学习与外显学习的协同效应。36名大一男生、30名初二男生被分别随机分入先外显学习后类比学习组、先类比学习后外显学习组、类比学习与外显学习交替进行组,评价各组在保持测试和迁移测试中的绩效(动作准确性、动作评分和外显知识数量)。结果表明,不同年龄青少年初学者学习网球技能时,类比学习与外显学习的最佳组合方式不同。对于大学生初学者而言,类比学习与外显学习交替进行组的绩效好于先外显学习后类比学习组和先类比学习后外显学习组;对于初中生初学者而言,先类比学习后外显学习组的绩效好于类比学习与外显学习交替进行组和先外显学习后类比学习组。这启示我们,应根据学习者的不同年龄选用不同的组合方式以获得更好的学习效果。对这一问题的探讨将为提高日常体育教学、运动训练以及全民健身指导的效率提供理论支撑,为内隐与外显的协同学习理论在运动技能学习中的实践性操作提供建设性的指导意见,进一步丰富国内、外运动技能内隐学习领域的研究成果。  相似文献   

4.
网球技能学习中类比学习与外显学习协同效应的实验研究   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
采用实验法,探讨大学生网球初学者在学习正手击球时类比学习与外显学习的协同效应.受试被随机分为外显学习组、类比学习组、类比与外显学习结合组,以击球准确性、动作评分和动作知识知晓数量为指标,对3组受试进行评价.结果显示:在保持测试中,外显与类比学习结合组的击球准确性和动作评分好于类比学习组和外显学习组;在迁移测试中,外显与类比学习结合组和类比学习组的击球准确性和动作评分好于外显学习组;在知晓动作知识的评价中,类比与外显学习结合组习得的动作知识数量多于类比学习组和外显学习组,而外显学习组多于类比学习组.  相似文献   

5.
采用实验法,探讨类比学习与外显学习的不同组合方式对大学生初学者学习不同性质网球技能的影响。受试被随机分为闭锁性技能任务组和开放性技能任务组,再将两组受试随机分为外显-类比学习组、类比-外显学习组和类比与外显学习同时进行组。以击球准确性、动作评分和有关动作的知识数量为指标,对受试的绩效进行评价。结果显示,学习闭锁性技能时,同时进行组受试的击球准确性、动作评分和动作知识的掌握程度比其他两组好;学习开放性技能时,类比-外显学习组受试的击球准确性、动作评分和动作知识的掌握程度比其他两组好。  相似文献   

6.
探讨开放性运动技能学习中类比学习与外显学习的最佳组合方式。36名大学一年级男生被随机分入外显-类比学习组、类比-外显学习组以及类比与外显学习同时进行组。以击球准确性、动作评分和动作知识数量为指标,对受试的绩效进行了评价。结果显示,大学是初学者学习开放性网球技能时,类比-外显学习组受试的击球准确性、动作评分和动作知识的掌握程度比其它两组好。  相似文献   

7.
采用实验法,探讨类比与外显学习的不同组合方式对大学生网球初学者学习不同难度技能的影响。将右利手男生随机分为高、低难度任务组,再分为先外显后类比学习组、先类比后外显学习组、类比与外显学习交替组,以击球准确性、动作评分和动作知识数量为指标,对受试的发球绩效进行评价。结果显示:在学习高难度发球技能时,先类比后外显学习组的绩效好于其他两组;在学习低难度发球技能时,类比与外显学习交替组的绩效好于其他两组。  相似文献   

8.
探讨高校学生采用内隐方式对乒乓球技能学习及学习兴趣的影响,以杭州电子科技大学2008级乒乓球班女生为对象,设实验和对照2个组,实验组采用内隐学习方法,对照组采用外显式学习方法。结果表明:①在两种不同的教学方法中,掌握单项技术上未发生显著性差异;②两种学习方式在对大学生体育学习兴趣方面都起到了正面的效果,单内隐学习在这方面的表现要明显优于外显学习。  相似文献   

9.
使用目前最有效的强分离程序--匹配(内隐学习)和编辑(外显学习)研究范式,抽取大二普通系学生120名,采用3×2随机设计方差分析.探讨篮球战术决策获得过程中,内隐学习与外显学习的关系及情境复杂性对内隐学习与外显学习的影响效应.结果显示:在篮球战术决策获得过程中,内隐学习和外显学习及混合学习均显示出显著的绩效,并且内隐与外显学习相结合产生了促进、协同效应.在篮球战术决策获得过程中,内隐学习和外显学习与情境复杂性发现有交互作用,即在低复杂性情境下,内隐学习优于外显学习;高复杂性情境下,外显学习优于内隐学习.作为篮球战术决策研究指标最有效、可靠的是决策正确性和决策速度,决策自信心水平作为协变量在实验中没有影响篮球战术决策.  相似文献   

10.
乒乓球项目中内隐学习效果的探讨   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
采用序列学习范式的方法,引用到乒乓球运动项目,依据发球机制定了序列学习的规则知识,考察了乒乓球项目中内隐学习效应的存在性,并探讨了在乒乓球运动项目中内隐学习在不同重复概率条件下和应激条件下学习的效果。结果表明,在乒乓球运动项目中存在内隐学习;在模糊的规则条件下内隐学习的效果要好于外显学习;应激条件下,主要影响两组被试操作成绩的是非规则知识的发挥。  相似文献   

11.
双任务中内隐运动技能学习对提高保持成绩的作用   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
本实验研究的目的是,调查在双任务条件下一个运动系列能否被不觉察地掌握,内隐学习是否比外显学习更有效地增加保持.36名实验对象被随机分为内隐学习组或外显学习组.要求所有被试通过敲击一个键盘上的F和J键,来移动计算机屏幕上的横杆,去接住一个下落的小球.一个固定的5种小球下落曲线的系列被安插在每组15次练习的中段.同时,实验参加者需要数每组练习中的所听到的嘟嘟声的数目,以作为第二项任务.在掌握该技能后的24小时进行3次保持测验.ANOVA分析发现固定小球下落曲线段的接球成绩随着练习的增加而提高,但是练习对随机段没有作用.尽管两种练习条件下,运动技能学习成绩没有差异,但内隐学习组比外显学习组在模拟接球的保持测试和第二项任务中表现较准确.这些结果显示内隐学习是一个改善运动技能长期记忆的有效手段,并且与外显学习相比,它在练习中占用较少的认知资源.  相似文献   

12.
The aim of this study was to ascertain whether the performances of implicit and explicit learners would converge over an extended period of learning. Participants practised a complex motor skill - golf putting - for 3000 trials, either with a concurrent secondary, tone-counting task (implicit learning) or without such a task (explicit learning). The cognitive demands of the secondary task were predicted to prevent the accumulation of verbalizable rules about the motor task. The implicit group reported significantly fewer rules than the explicit group on subsequent verbal protocols. The performance of the implicit group remained below that of the explicit group throughout the learning phase. However, no significant differences were found between groups during a delayed retention test. Additionally, for the participants in the explicit group only, a Reinvestment Scale score correlated positively with the number of rules accrued and negatively with overall putting performance during the learning phase. We use the results to argue against the excessive use of verbal instruction during skill acquisition, which might be unnecessary and ultimately might hamper performance under stressful conditions.  相似文献   

13.
From novice to no know-how: a longitudinal study of implicit motor learning   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
The aim of this study was to ascertain whether the performances of implicit and explicit learners would converge over an extended period of learning. Participants practised a complex motor skill--golf putting--for 3000 trials, either with a concurrent secondary, tone-counting task (implicit learning) or without such a task (explicit learning). The cognitive demands of the secondary task were predicted to prevent the accumulation of verbalizable rules about the motor task. The implicit group reported significantly fewer rules than the explicit group on subsequent verbal protocols. The performance of the implicit group remained below that of the explicit group throughout the learning phase. However, no significant differences were found between groups during a delayed retention test. Additionally, for the participants in the explicit group only, a Reinvestment Scale score correlated positively with the number of rules accrued and negatively with overall putting performance during the learning phase. We use the results to argue against the excessive use of verbal instruction during skill acquisition, which might be unnecessary and ultimately might hamper performance under stressful conditions.  相似文献   

14.

We examined the relationship between implicit and explicit “exerciser” and “sedentary” self-identity when activated by stereotypes. Undergraduate participants (N = 141) wrote essays about university students who either liked to exercise or engage in sedentary activities. This was followed by an implicit identity task and an explicit measure of exercise self-identity. Results showed that implicit and explicit exerciser identities were not highly correlated. There were also no significant prime effects, but women showed greater implicit sedentary identity, whereas men showed greater implicit exercise identity. This research suggests that implicit exercise-related identity is a distinct construct from explicit exercise identity. The results also reflect responses to societal pressures for women to be thin and for men to be strong, when free of self-presentational bias.  相似文献   

15.
Two experiments were conducted to examine the hypothesis that learning by analogy will invoke characteristics of an implicit mode of motor learning. In the first experiment, table tennis novices learned to hit forehand topspin implicitly, explicitly or by analogy. The results showed that the analogy and implicit learning groups accumulated equivalently fewer explicit rules than the explicit learning group during the learning phase. When a concurrent secondary task was added, the explicit learning group suffered from a significantly more serious performance impairment than the analogy and implicit learning groups; no significant differences were seen between the latter two groups. Self-perceived performance was correlated to actual performance in the explicit learning group but not in the analogy or the implicit learning groups. In the second experiment, the performance of an explicit learning group was found to be impaired by both a stress intervention and a thought suppression intervention, whereas the performance of an analogy learning group was not. These characteristics of analogy learning parallel those reported in the implicit learning literature, suggesting that analogy learning may be an effective method for teaching skills implicitly in sport.  相似文献   

16.
Two experiments were conducted to examine the hypothesis that learning by analogy will invoke characteristics of an implicit mode of motor learning. In the first experiment, table tennis novices learned to hit forehand topspin implicitly, explicitly or by analogy. The results showed that the analogy and implicit learning groups accumulated equivalently fewer explicit rules than the explicit learning group during the learning phase. When a concurrent secondary task was added, the explicit learning group suffered from a significantly more serious performance impairment than the analogy and implicit learning groups; no significant differences were seen between the latter two groups. Self-perceived performance was correlated to actual performance in the explicit learning group but not in the analogy or the implicit learning groups. In the second experiment, the performance of an explicit learning group was found to be impaired by both a stress intervention and a thought suppression intervention, whereas the performance of an analogy learning group was not. These characteristics of analogy learning parallel those reported in the implicit learning literature, suggesting that analogy learning may be an effective method for teaching skills implicitly in sport.  相似文献   

17.
The benefits of implicit and explicit motor learning approaches in young adults have been studied extensively, but much less in children. This study investigated the relationship between fundamental motor ability and implicit/explicit learning in children using the errorless learning paradigm. First, the motor ability of 261 children (142 boys, 119 girls) aged 9–12 years (M?=?9.74, SD?=?0.67) was measured. Second, children with motor ability scores in the upper and lower quartile learned a golf-putting skill in either an errorless (implicit) or errorful (explicit) learning condition. Four groups were formed: Errorless High-Ability (n?=?13), Errorless Low-Ability (n?=?11), Errorful High-Ability (n?=?10), and Errorful Low-Ability (n?=?11). Learning consisted of 300 practice trials, while testing included a 50-trial retention test, followed by a 50-trial secondary task transfer test, and another 50-trial retention test. The results showed that for high- and low-ability errorless learners, motor performance was unaffected by the secondary task, as was the case for high-ability errorful learners. Low-ability errorful learners performed worse with a secondary task and were significantly poorer than the corresponding high-ability group. These results suggest that implicit motor learning (errorless) may be beneficial for children with low motor ability. The findings also show a trend that children of high motor ability might benefit from learning explicitly (errorful). Further research is recommended to examine the compatibility of implicit and explicit approaches for children of different abilities.  相似文献   

18.
运动技能的内隐学习与分心练习——对应激的抵御   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
为抵御应激对运动技能水平发挥的影响 ,研究以轨迹追踪任务为实验范式 ,将 32名被试分 4组在不同条件下进行为期 12 d的练习 ,并比较了各组在两种应激条件下的测试结果。结果显示 :对于复杂的操作任务 ,内隐学习比外显学习更有效。分心练习所获得运动技能较少受高应激的影响  相似文献   

19.
社会赞许能否调节内隐认知与外显认知的关系   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
自Greenwald(1995)[1]提出内隐联结测验以来,内隐认知成为社会认知研究的热点。在已往研究中,内隐认知(含内隐态度和内隐自尊)和外显认知(含外显态度和外显自尊)存在低相关,但是这个相关系数在一定范围内存在波动。本文将通过两个研究来探究这一波动背后的原因,即寻找影响两者关系的第三变量——社会赞许。研究一:探测了34名在校大学生的社会赞许倾向、内隐自尊和外显自尊。结果显示受社会赞许影响不同的人,内隐自尊和外显自尊相关系数存在数值上的高低差异,但这个差异没有达到显著效果。另外,受社会赞许影响不同的两组被试在外显自尊维度上存在显著差异。研究二:探测了45名体育大学优秀运动员对兴奋剂和普通饮料的内隐、外显态度。结果显示对服用兴奋剂和服用普通饮料这两种受社会赞许影响不同的行为,被试的内隐和外显态度的相关有细微差异,但这个差异也没有达到显著效果。另外,这两种行为态度,在外显态度的维度上存在显著差异。上述两个研究的结果显示社会赞许对内隐认知和外显认知的关系的调节作用不显著,文中讨论了这一结果产生的原因。  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号